Search training for people with visual field loss after stroke: a cohort study Short title



Download 211.28 Kb.
Page1/2
Date conversion16.05.2018
Size211.28 Kb.
  1   2
Search training for people with visual field loss after stroke: a cohort study

Short title – Search training in occupational therapy

Ailie J Turton, Senior Lecturer, Department of Allied Health Professions, University of the West of England, Bristol

Jayne Angilley, Clinical Specialist Occupational Therapist Stroke, Stroke Therapy Team, Camborne and Redruth Community Hospital, Cornwall Partnership NHS Foundation Trust

Verity Longley, PhD Student, School of Psychological Sciences, University of Manchester

Philip Clatworthy, Consultant Stroke Neurologist, North Bristol NHS Trust and Stroke Association Thompson Family Senior Clinical Lecturer, University of Bristol

Iain D Gilchrist, Professor of Neuropsychology, School of Experimental Psychology, University of Bristol




For correspondence:

Dr Ailie Turton,

University of the West of England,

Glenside Campus, Blackberry Hill,

Bristol, BS16 1DD

(ailie.turton@uwe.ac.uk)


Abstract

Background

People with visual field loss after stroke often experience difficulties in everyday activities. The purpose of this study was to assess the acceptability of search training as used within occupational therapy and the feasibility of possible measures for use in a future trial.


Methods:

Nine participants took part in a goal oriented intervention that was delivered three times a week for three weeks. Patient reports of acceptability and outcomes using the Visual Function Questionnaire-25 (VFQ-25) were collected. Participants’ room search behaviour before and after the intervention was recorded using a head worn camera.



Results:

Eight participants completed nine treatment visits. All participants reported improved awareness and attention to the blind side during activities following the intervention. 7/8 participants change scores on VFQ-25 exceeded 6 points. Patterns of head-direction behaviour and overall room search times were variable across patients; markedly improved performance was only evident in the most severely affected participant.



Conclusions:

The intervention was acceptable. The VFQ-25 is a feasible measure for assessing patient reported outcomes. While the room search was informative about individuals’ behaviour, more sophisticated methods of gaze tracking would allow search processes to be determined in real world activities that are relevant to patients’ goals.


Key Words: occupational therapy, stroke, hemianopia, intervention; measurement

Introduction

An estimated 60% of stroke patients lose part of their visual field or have eye movement problems, with 35% completely losing one half of their visual field (Ali et al. 2013). These deficits often persist leading to long term limitations in activities of daily living (ADL). In one survey people with hemianopia reported poor performance in grooming, eating, mobility, reading, shopping, money management, driving, work and leisure (Warren, 2009). Hemianopia also causes people to bump into things when walking (Gilhotra et al. 2002) and increases the risk of falls (Ramrattan et al. 2001; Czernuszenko and Czlonkowska 2009).

These limitations may in part be explained by inefficient visual scanning and searching (Warren 2009). Laboratory based studies have shown that some patients with visual field deficits move their eyes to scan a display in a disorganised way. They make more repeat visits to visual targets than is normal and their eye movements towards the blind field have a reduced amplitude. Patients tend to miss some stimuli and have slower performance than controls in finding targets (Meienberg et al 1981; Zihl 1995). Patients looking at images demonstrate some intrinsic compensation for visual field loss; patients spent more time exploring the area of the picture corresponding to the blind hemifield than the seeing side (Pambakian et al. 2000). Problems are exacerbated in patients with visual field deficits who also have visuo-spatial neglect. Studies of search behaviour in patients with neglect show that they fail to explore the side of space contralateral to the cerebral hemisphere affected by the stroke and in searching they frequently revisit stimuli on the ipsilateral side (Behrmann et al. 2004). Patients with both field loss and neglect have the poorest search performance (Behrmann et al. 2004). Visual search is an integral process within many ADLs; for example, in finding items on the supermarket shelves, or avoiding hazards when walking along the pavement. If the search and scanning behaviour observed in laboratory tasks is carried over to visual function in everyday life, it is easy to see how occupational performance may be affected.

The consequences of visual field loss are very often long-lasting. Six months after onset, people with hemianopia may be much more limited than people with other post-stroke impairments in terms of social functioning, role limitations, and emotional well-being (Gall et al. 2010). Despite this there has been no large-scale clinical trial of rehabilitation for visual field loss after stroke. The recent UK National Clinical Guidelines for Stroke (2016) fall short of recommending or even mentioning any specific treatment. The most recent Cochrane review found only limited evidence that training compensatory visual search strategies may improve scanning and reading performance, and insufficient high quality evidence of effectiveness of treatments for improving independence in ADL (Pollock et al., 2011a). These conclusions are not significantly altered by the number of small studies that have evaluated different interventions since publication of this review (Gillen et al 2015). There is a recognised need to find better ways to help people with visual problems after stroke. The James Lind Alliance priority setting exercise for stroke rehabilitation research in Scotland has ‘What are the best ways to treat visual problems after stroke?’ at number five in its top ten research questions (Pollock et al 2012).

Current interventions, if offered, comprise giving information and directing patients to openly available computer based scanning and searching exercises (Rowe et al 2016; personal observations: Clatworthy http://hemianopia.blogspot.co.uk/p/for-patients.html ). While computerised training offers a cheap means for self-directed learning, it is uncertain whether learning these abstract search tasks carries over to improve performance in ADL. Visual learning effects tend to be very task specific (Schuett et al 2012); it therefore makes sense to train visual searching within occupations. Using their core skills in assessing and facilitating occupational performance, Occupational Therapists are well placed to provide task specific vision training. Visual search training is sometimes included within occupational therapy for people with visual problems (Pollock et al 2011b). However, opportunities to practice extended ADLs are limited in hospital, therefore occupational therapists working in community services should be more active in helping people to improve their visual performance in everyday living. A previous study of an occupational therapy intervention (table top activities), delivered in an outpatient setting, showed promise for improving participants’ psychological adjustment to living with visual field loss (Taylor et al. 2011). However, this intervention was not goal oriented and it is not yet known whether visual scanning and search training in occupational therapy is effective for improving occupational performance in everyday living.

Prior to testing effectiveness of complex interventions, the UK Medical Research Council recommends defining and modelling the intervention and testing its feasibility for delivery (Craig et al. 2008). We previously described an intervention for hemianopia, based on the practice of a Specialist Community based Occupational Therapist (Turton et al. 2015). Using this description, an intervention guide was written giving the rationale for visual search training using valued activities and process activities. In the present study, we wished to determine the feasibility and acceptability of the intervention to participants when delivered three times a week for three weeks by an Occupational Therapy Technician.

Another aspect of this modelling process is to determine the mechanisms by which the intervention works, in this case how participants’ search and scanning behaviour is changed as a result of the intervention. Methods for measuring patients’ behaviours while searching real environments are not well developed, though this has been done in healthy subjects (Land et al 1999) and in a single case study (Land et al, 2002). Visual exploration of scenes in the real world involve not only eye but also head and whole body movements, particularly when shifting the point of gaze by more than ten degrees of visual angle (Land et al 1999; Guitton and Vole 1987; Freedman 2008). A method for measuring where the person is looking with respect to a scene would potentially provide information about how an intervention works. Such measurement might include the search strategies adopted, learning rates, retention and generalisation of learning to untrained tasks. Eye trackers are commonly used in laboratories or other controlled environments (Howard et al. 2011; 2013), however eye trackers are delicate and expensive. Assessing where people are looking in the wide field searches that are predominant in daily living may not require the precision of eye trackers. Because large gaze shifts involve movements of the head and trunk, a measurement of head position relative to the scene provides a proxy for gaze position. In this study we tested the suitability of using an inexpensive head worn camera to record search behaviour of participants before and after receiving the occupational therapy intervention.

A further part of the modelling process is to determine suitability of relevant outcome measures that could be used in a definitive trial. For this purpose, we selected the US National Eye Institute’s Visual Function Questionnaire (VQF-25), a patient reported outcome measure comprising vision related and social constructs (Mangione et al 2001). The VFQ-25 was not developed specifically for stroke (Hepworth et al 2015), but it has good reliability and construct validity in patients with age related macular degeneration (Revicki et al 2010). We wanted to see whether the questionnaire would be able to measure change in people with visual field loss due to stroke.

In summary, our modelling phase study had three aims:


  1. To test the acceptability of a three-week home based occupational therapy programme for training visual search within the context of participant’s goals in everyday living tasks

  2. To evaluate a room search task for its utility in determining search behaviour in the sample

  3. To test the VFQ-25 as a measure of change after intervention

Method

This was a small cohort study. Approval was obtained from South West Frenchay NHS Research Ethics Committee (ref. no. 11/SW/0306). All participants gave written informed consent. Written consent was also obtained from other adults in the household since the room search video recordings were made in their homes.



Participants

Inclusion criteria:



  • diagnosis of stroke (physician’s judgement)

  • to be discharged to home

  • performance in ADL affected by visual search problems (Occupational Therapist’s judgement)

  • failed at least one of the confrontation test (visual fields) (Pattern 1996) and Star Cancellation task from the Behavioural Inattention Test (Wilson et al.1987)

  • judged as able to participate in the research processes and intervention

  • able to consent to participation

Exclusion criteria:

  • previous history of severe sight impairment

  • previous history of disability affecting personal care (modified Rankin score 4 or more) (van Swieten et al. 1988)

  • under 18 years old

  • discharged to a nursing home or to residential care.

Consecutive service users fitting the eligibility criteria were identified by a community Stroke Care Co-ordinator and from the local hospital’s stroke rehabilitation ward. Participants were recruited by Stroke Research Network Officers in the local acute stroke service or by the research Occupational Therapist in the community. Demographic characteristics of participants were collected: age, gender, side of stroke, lesion site, time since stroke, motor and sensory deficits, results of vision examination including perimetry (orthoptist’s report).

Recruitment took place from 1 January 2013 to 31st January 2014.



Intervention

The main principle of the intervention is that the search training should be specific to and tailored for the participants’ goal occupations. Participants learn to systematically search all relevant areas of the environment for the task, especially to their blind side. Before commencing the intervention an assessment, goal setting and planning visit was carried out by the Research Occupational Therapist. She used her own vision assessments and goal setting procedure. Following this visit, participants received an intensive course of scanning and search training using various process activities and occupations. Task specific learning requires intensive practice and therefore it was decided that the intervention should be delivered in three one hour visits each week for three weeks. The therapy programme for this study was delivered by two part-time Occupational Therapy Technicians who were supervised by the Research Specialist Occupational Therapist. The intervention guide, written for use in the study, provided direction and examples indicating: how to educate the participant about the effects of their visual problems on ADL performance and the need for compensatory strategies; the use of systematic scanning within the context of meaningful and goal specific activities; the use of process (or remedial) activities to intensively train search strategies, for example beginning to search a table top or room on the blind side and to systematically search back towards the seeing side; grading difficulty by manipulating distractors and the area of space for the task (Turton et al 2015). One or two ADLs were selected for practice according to the participant’s goals. Examples include preparing food or drinks, playing a card game, or steering a powered wheelchair, or outdoor tasks such as walking in the street or shopping. To demonstrate fidelity to the intervention the Occupational Therapy Technicians completed structured treatment logs giving details of activities used, their purpose and examples of search strategies and comments on participants’ performance.

Participants were encouraged to record their experiences of occupational therapy visits, to act as a reminder when asked to answer a structured questionnaire about their perceptions of the intervention. This questionnaire was completed either by telephone, or face to face if time allowed. It comprised closed questions about the acceptability of the frequency of visits, and whether the benefits justified the effort required to participate. Participants were asked to rate how beneficial they perceived the intervention to be on an 11-point numerical scale (0 being not at all beneficial, 10 being extremely beneficial). They were also asked what difference they thought the intervention had made to them and what they liked and disliked about it. While it is acknowledged that having the therapy team ask the questions risked biasing participants’ responses, the advantage was that the team would gain a more detailed understanding of how the intervention might be improved.

Room search process measurement

[figure 1 near here]

The room search task was developed by the research team for this study. A small, familiar, unobtrusive object belonging to the participant was searched for, for example a key on a fob. The task was standardised as much as possible within the participant’s home setting. A Go-Pro digital video camera was positioned squarely on the participant’s head and secured with an elastic strap (figure 1 inset). The position of the camera was aligned using a laser pointer to ensure the camera was facing straight ahead. The participant was seated centrally against one wall of the room; giving scope for searching the room directly in front of them and to their extreme left and right sides. The tester divided the room mentally into eight sections from the point of view of the participant; no physical markers were used. The facing wall and area in front of the participant was divided into central left upper and lower quadrants and each peripheral wall or space was divided into upper and lower halves (see figure 1). Sixteen searches were performed with the object being placed twice in each of the eight sections. The order in which locations were used across trials was random.

The participant was seated with eyes closed while the object was very quietly placed to minimise auditory cues. The participant was told when to open eyes to start each search. Each search was recorded from the head camera and timed manually using a stopwatch. Searches were stopped after 120 seconds if the object had not been found, to prevent frustration, demoralisation or boredom. The room search task was piloted on a sample of ten healthy volunteers who searched symmetrically across the rooms used (Turton, 2014, pp15-17).

The room search task was carried out at the first assessment visit and in the week after completion of the intervention. The task was administered by the Research Occupational Therapist, with the help of an Occupational Therapy Technician. Resources did not allow for an independent assessor.

VFQ25

The VFQ-25 was administered on the assessment visits before and after the intervention by the Research Occupational Therapist. The VFQ-25 represents 11 vision related constructs: general vision, ocular pain, near vision, distance vision, driving, peripheral vision, colour vision, role limitations, dependency, social function, and mental health, plus a general health question. Rasch analysis has suggested that the measure may have better internal consistency without the items for general health, pain around the eyes, and driving, and that its constructs could be collapsed into visual functioning and socio-emotional scales, respectively (Marella et al 2010). The minimal important difference of the total composite VFQ-25 score was 6.13 points in a study of patients with diabetic macular oedema (Lloyd et al. 2013).



Data Analysis

To indicate feasibility, the number of intervention visits was counted and the tasks and activities summarised and compared with the intended number and scope of the visits. For assessment of acceptability, responses to the participants’ questionnaire about the intervention were collated.

Video recordings of the room search task were independently coded by a university researcher. Sports analysis video player software (Kinovea), was used to create cross hairs which were aligned to the centre of point of view of the camera and therefore the direction that the head was pointing. With reference to a diagram showing the room divisions (as in figure 1), the video was played slowly so that the assessor could follow the cross hairs as it played and record the head-direction sequence on the diagram. A new head direction was coded when the camera crossed into a new segment, for example “upper left, upper central left, upper central right”, etc. ‘Head direction’ refers to the position in space to which the centre of the head is pointing; in natural viewing this approximates the region in space to which the eyes and so the centre of the visual field is directed. The number of head-directions in each section of the room was calculated for each trial and summed across all 16 trials. From this a proportion of head-directions in each half of the room was calculated. In addition, the first five head-directions for each trial were used to count the early search head-directions on each side of the room (blind or seeing side). For these frequencies, we calculated 95% confidence intervals for each patient. The durations of trials were summarised as medians with interquartile ranges. A note was made of the trials truncated at 120 seconds and the position of the object in these failed searches.

In presenting the data we have taken a case series approach to preserve the diversity of the participant sample.

VFQ-25 scoring was carried out in accordance with the test manual: raw scores were recoded to percentages of the maximum scores and items within each subscale averaged together to create subscale scores; the average of the sub-scale scores provided an overall composite score. Change scores for each subsection were determined. As suggested by Marella et al (2010) items general health, pain around the eyes, and driving were excluded from the analysis.

Results

Fourteen participants were recruited to the study. However, five withdrew before participation: two having completely recovered; one was too ill, one was discharged into care and one no longer wanted to participate. Of the nine participants remaining five were male; ages ranged from 54 to 84; seven had hemianopia, the remaining two had partial visual field loss on one side; eight had left-sided visual loss; four had coexisting visual inattention. Participants’ characteristics are given in table 1. The goals and activities used are given in table 2. Activities were chosen according to participants’ goals. Six participants had goals that related to mobility (on foot or in a wheelchair), either inside or outside the home; four wanted to return to driving; two participants wanted to return to work; six had goals related to meal preparation; three wanted to improve their ability to work on a computer and three wanted to improve their reading. Since driving was not possible the goal to return to driving was addressed by focussing on improving awareness and scanning to the affected side over distances similar to those used when driving, when out walking or sitting in a stationary car.

[tables 1 & 2 near here]

Process training activities such as finding playing cards from an array on a table top or across the floor were used with all participants to increase insight into the effects of visual field loss and to provide intensive practice in using new search strategies before trying them out in goal tasks. Eight participants had all nine intervention visits; participant E received eight visits, after which he returned to work.



Participants’ views of the intervention

Eight participants completed the questionnaire giving their views of the intervention and perceived benefits (Table 3). All respondents found the number of visits and the treatment acceptable and thought the intervention worth the effort required.

[Table 3 near here]

[Figure 2 near here]



Room search results

Figure 2A shows the percentage of head-directions in the half of the room on participants’ blind side before and after intervention. All nine participants directed their head movements to both sides of the room. Figure 2A shows evidence of an increase in searching in the blind side following the intervention for participants C, F and G. Participant A appeared to search the blind field less following the intervention. Note that this participant demonstrated a strong bias towards searching the blind field rather than the good field before the intervention; given that the target is equally likely to be in either side of the room this change could be considered adaptive.

Figure 2B shows the same data as Figure 2A but for the first five head-directions only. Participant A again displays a strong bias to start the search on the blind side both before and after training. Participant G’s increase in searching the blind field after treatment is evident in this early search phase. Participants H and I show a dramatic decrease in early search of the blind side after treatment, but comparison with Figure 2A suggests this must be compensated for by searching the blind side later in the search process.

Figures 2C and 2D plot participants’ median search times and interquartile range (IQR). There is little data available here per participant so only qualitative comparisons are appropriate. Search times were very variable overall with a number of trials being stopped at the 120 second limit. Except for participant F, there is no clear link visible between head-direction behaviour and overall search times across patients.



VFQ-25

Table 4 shows participants’ composite scores before and after training with changes in composite and domain scores. Positive values represent improved scores. Participants’ assessments of their visual functioning ranged widely; composite percentage scores ranged from 20.3 to 83.6 before training. Participants A, B and G reported mild limitations; others reported more severe deficits and socio-emotional impact. Composite scores for all eight participants who completed the VFQ-25 after treatment were improved; in all of them except G this exceeded the estimated minimally important difference of 6.3. Gains after treatment were seen across domains.

[Table 4 near here]

Discussion

Goal specific visual search training was delivered to nine participants with stroke affecting visual functioning. This small sample contained a wide range of visual and physical functioning. Consequently, we applied visual search training to a diverse range of everyday activities.

The intervention utilised activities identified as goals by the participants and was delivered intensively in one hour visits, three times a week for three weeks by Occupational Therapy Technicians. This is more intensive than is usual in the local community occupational therapy service where the Research Occupational Therapist works, where visits to service users with visual problems after stroke are typically once a fortnight or once a month over several months. After three weeks of more intensive intervention the study participants reported increased awareness of their visual field loss, being better able to find things and carry out everyday activities. They found the frequency of visits acceptable and considered the benefits to be worth the work done. Occupational therapy is a complex intervention and may have differing effects and influences on different individuals. While the amount of training given was deemed acceptable by participants and intensity of practice is important for learning, the duration of the therapy programme could be varied according to individual need.

Accurate and sensitive methods for measuring visual search performance in everyday activities are needed to determine learning achieved. The room search task was designed to capture search behaviour in a familiar space. In training compensatory search strategies, we aim for more efficient and successful searching. The room search results showed that search behaviour varies between individuals. In concurrence with previous findings of intrinsic compensation in viewing images (Pambakian et al., 2000), our room search results showed that some individuals had already begun to spend more time searching on their blind side before the start of the intervention. Nevertheless, searching on the blind side was increased after the intervention in some patients, though this did not necessarily decrease search duration. While overall the room search results demonstrated considerable variation across participants, one participant stood out as having substantially improved success in searching; participant F showed remarkable improvements in the time taken to find objects to her blind side after treatment. This participant was the most impaired in terms of inattention and eye movements; she was unable to direct her gaze to the left and was severely physically impaired. Four months post stroke she was dependent in personal care and transfers and could not move around her home. The goal directed intervention included searching for clothes in her wardrobe, kitchen activities and driving an electric wheelchair. Although we cannot be certain her improved performance was not due to spontaneous recovery (Farne et al. 2004), it is likely to have been influenced by the opportunity the intervention provided to intensively learn to explore her environment.

The room search task proved to be useful in investigating search behaviour in a real world space and was low cost and robust. This is an important departure from tests used in clinical practice where performance is usually confined to narrow field tasks in near space using computer screens or pencil and paper cancellation tasks (Pollock et al 2011a). We believe the room search task represents a significant and important improvement on current process measures. However, it still suffers from a number of limitations: It was a stationary task and was very deliberately used to test performance, without the distractions of real ADLs. Consequently, participants’ performance may not reflect their behaviour in real life. This static task may not be very informative about activities in which the participant is moving about, for example walking around outside or in shops, driving a wheelchair or in kitchen activities.

Different real world tasks have different speed and accuracy requirements and perhaps different search strategies should be encouraged accordingly. The intervention was substantially goal directed and performance measures should be relevant to the tasks that the intervention is aiming to influence. Camera technology is improving: in the short space of time since we started this study affordable spectacles with digital video cameras set into the bridge have become readily available. More sophisticated software for mapping gaze to the environment are also developing (Leelasawassuk, et al. 2015; Lukierski, et al. 2017). Currently the technology cannot reliably capture where the viewer is looking in busy environments or poorly lit conditions, but once these challenges are met, understanding search patterns in everyday life may be more accessible and become part of process measurement in occupational therapy practice.

The VFQ-25 measured change after the intervention across participants. Change scores indicated probable clinically important improvements, suggesting greater confidence in everyday living and participation. Of course, these perceived gains may not have been due to the intervention; there was no control group nor methods to reduce bias. Natural recovery, increased attention from the research therapy team, the familiarity and possible bias of the administrator, or other influences may have affected the change in VFQ-25 scores. However, the results suggest the VFQ-25 could be a useful outcome measure in a larger randomised control trial.

In conclusion, this study has established that this visual search training programme tested is feasible and acceptable within community based occupational therapy and was generally liked by participants. The VFQ-25 appears to be a good candidate for an outcome measure in a future clinical trial. We have taken a step forwards towards evaluating the intervention and determining an appropriate outcome measure for a future trial.



Key findings

  • A community based occupational therapy intervention to train visual search in people affected by stroke is acceptable and feasible.

  • A room search task was informative about individuals’ performance, but more sophisticated methods are needed to evaluate search processes in a variety of real world activities.

What the study has added

The study has shown that this intervention is acceptable and feasible. It has also demonstrated measurement of search in the home context.



Acknowledgements

Laura Paterson and Sue Reynolds, Peninsula Community Health Occupational Therapy Service, delivered the intervention, with supervision from Jayne Angilley. We are grateful to the Therapy Services manager, Gill Mead, for her support for this project. Thanks to Dr Iris Terrer Dimwadyo for providing independent analysis of the room search videos. Last but not least, our warmest thanks to the participants.



Ethics approval

Approval for the studies was obtained from South West Frenchay NHS Research Ethics Committee (ref. no. 11/SW/0306, Occupational Therapy Vision After Stroke 28/10/2011)



Declaration of conflicting interests

The Authors confirm that there is no conflict of interest



Funding

This study was funded by a Research Priority Grant from the UK Occupational Therapy Research Foundation.



References

Ali M, Hazelton C, Lyden P, et al. (2013) Recovery from poststroke visual impairment: Evidence from a clinical trials resource. Neurorehabilitation and Neural Repair 27(2): 133–141.

Behrmann M, Ebert P and Black SE (2004) Hemispatial neglect and visual search: a large scale analysis. Cortex 40, 247-263

Craig P, Dieppe P, Macintyre S, et al. (2008) Developing and evaluating complex interventions: the new Medical Research Council guidance. British Medical Journal 337: 979-983.

Czernuszenko A and Czlonkowska A (2009) Risk factors for falls in stroke patients during inpatient rehabilitation. Clinical Rehabilitation 23: 176-188.

Farnè A, Buxbaum LJ, Ferraro M, et al (2004), Patterns of spontaneous recovery of neglect and associated disorders in acute right brain-damaged patients Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery & Psychiatry 75:1401-1410.

Freedman EG (2008) Coordination of the eyes and head during orienting. Experimental Brain Research 190: 369-387.

Gall C, Franke GH and Sabel BA (2010) Vision-related quality of life in first stroke patients with homonymous visual field defects. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 8: 33. Published online 2010 Mar 26. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-8-33

Gilhotra JS, Mitchell P, Healey PR, et al. (2002) Homonymous visual field defects and stroke in an older population. Stroke 33: 2417-2420.

Gillen G, Nilsen DM, Attridge J, et al. (2014) Effectiveness of interventions to improve occupational performance of people with cognitive impairments after stroke: An evidence-based review. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 69 Published online: 6901180040p1-6901180040p9. doi:10.5014/ajot.2015.012138

Guitton D and Volle M (1987) Gaze Control in Humans - Eye-head coordination during orienting movements to targets within and beyond the oculomotor range. Journal of Neurophysiology 58, 427-459.

Hepworth LR, Rowe FJ, Harper R, et al. (2015) Patient reported outcome measures for visual impairment after stroke: a systematic review. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 13:146 Published online 2015 Sep 15. doi: 10.1186/s12955-015-0338-x

Howard CJ, Pharaon RG, Körner C, et al. (2011) Visual search in the real world: Evidence for the formation of distractor representations. Perception 40, 1143-1153.

Howard CJ, Troscianko T, Gilchrist ID, et al. (2013) Suspiciousness perception in dynamic scenes: a comparison of CCTV operators and novices. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience 7, 441. Published online 2013 Aug 22. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00441

Land MF, Mennie N and Rusted J (1999) The roles of vision and eye movements in the control of activities of daily living. Perception 28: 1311-1328

Land MF, Furneaux, SM and Gilchrist ID. (2002). The organisation of visually mediated actions in a subject without eye movements Neurocase 8, 80-87.

Leelasawassuk T, Damen D and Mayol-Cuevas WW (2015). Estimating Visual Attention from a Head Mounted IMU. In: ISWC '15 Proceedings of the 2015 ACM International Symposium on Wearable Computers. Osaka, Japan — September 07 - 11, 2015 ISBN 978-1-4503-3578-2, pp. 147–150. Association for Computing Machinery New York USA

Lloyd AJ, Loftus J, Turner M, et al. (2013) Psychometric validation of the Visual Function Questionnaire-25 in patients with diabetic macular edema. Health and Quality of Life Outcomes 11:10. Published online 2013 Jan 24. doi: 10.1186/1477-7525-11-10

Lukierski R, Leutenegger S and Davidson AJ (2017) Room layout estimation from rapid omnidirectional exploration. IEEE International Conference on Robotics and Automation (ICRA) Singapore 29 May-3 June 2017 IEEE Xplore Digital Library. Available at: http://ieeexplore.ieee.org/abstract/document/7989747/?part=1 (accessed 29 Aug 2017)]

Mangione CM, Lee PP, Gutierrez PR, et al. (2001) Development of the 25-item National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire (VFQ-25), Archives of Ophthalmology 119, 1050-1058

Meienberg O, Zangemeister WH, Rosenberg M, et al. (1981) Saccadic eye movement strategies in patients with homonymous hemianopia. Annals of Neurology 9:537–44.

Marella M, Pesudovs K, Keeffe JE, et al. (2010) The Psychometric Validity of the NEI VFQ-25 for Use in a Low-Vision Population. Investigative Ophthalmology & Visual Science 51, 2878-2884

National Clinical Guidelines for Stroke (2016) Available at https://www.rcplondon.ac.uk/guidelines-policy/stroke-guidelines (accessed 23 January 2017)

Pambakian ALM, Wooding DS, Patel N, et al. (2000) Scanning the visual world: a study of patients with homonymous hemianopia Journal of Neurology Neurosurgery and Psychiatry 69: 751-759

Pattern J (1996) Neurological differential diagnosis, Heidelberg: Springer-Verlag, pp18.

Pollock A, Hazelton C, Henderson CA, et al. (2011a) Interventions for visual field defects in patients with stroke. Cochrane Database of Systematic Reviews 2011, Issue 10. Art. No.: CD008388.

Pollock A, Hazelton C, Brady M (2011b) Visual Problems After Stroke: A survey of current practice by Occupational Therapists working in UK stroke inpatient settings. Topics in Stroke Rehabilitation 18 (Suppl 1): 643–653

Pollock A, St George B, Fenton M, et al. (2012) Top ten research priorities relating to life after stroke. Lancet Neurology, 11 (3) 209 [also at James Lind Alliance, Stroke in Scotland Priority Setting Partnership http://www.jla.nihr.ac.uk/priority-setting-partnerships/stroke-in-scotland/top-10-priorities/ (accessed 23 January 2017)

Ramrattan RS, Wolfs RC, Panda-Jonas S, et al. (2001) Prevalence and causes of visual field loss in the elderly and associations with impairment in daily functioning: The Rotterdam Study. Archives of Ophthalmology 119: 1788-94

Revicki DA, Rentz AM, Harnam N, et al. (2010) Reliability and validity of the National Eye Institute Visual Function Questionnaire-25 in patients with age-related macular degeneration. Investigative Ophthalmology and Visual Science 51(2):712-7.

Rowe F, Walker M, Rockliffe J, et al. (2016) Delivery of high quality stroke and vision care: experiences of UK services. Disability and Rehabilitation 38 (8): 813-817

Schuett S, Heywood CA, Kentridge RW, et al. (2012) Rehabilitation of reading and visual exploration in visual field disorders: transfer or specificity? Brain,135(3): 912-21.

Taylor L, Poland F, Harrison P, et al. (2011) A quasi-experimental feasibility study to determine the effect of a systematic treatment programme on the scores of the Nottingham Adjustment Scale of individuals with visual field deficits following stroke. Clinical Rehabilitation 25: 43–50.

Turton A (2014) Description and measurement of visual scanning training in Occupational Therapy for patients with visual search deficits following stroke. Final Project Report College of Occupational Therapists and the University of the West of England. Available at http://eprints.uwe.ac.uk/30808/ (accessed 27 Jan 2017)

Turton AJ, Angilley J, Chapman M, et al., (2015) Visual search training in Occupational Therapy – an example of expert practice in community based stroke rehabilitation, British Journal of Occupational Therapy 78: 674-687

van Swieten JC, Koudstaal PJ, Visser MC, et al. (1988) Interobserver Agreement for the Assessment of Handicap in Stroke Patients. Stroke 19: 604-607.

Warren M (2009) Pilot study on activities of daily living limitations in adults with hemianopsia. American Journal of Occupational Therapy 63: 626–633.

Wilson B, Cockburn J and Halligan P (1987) Development of a behavioural test of visuospatial neglect. Archives of Physical Medicine and Rehabilitation 68: 98-102.

Zihl J. (1995) Visual scanning behaviour in patients with homonymous hemianopia. Neuropsychologia 33: 287–303.

Table 1. Participants

ID.

Gender

Age

Stroke lesion site

Months post stroke

Vision assessment: impairment and reported impact

A

Male


64

left temporo-occipital infarct

7

Right homonymous hemianopia, good corrected acuity for near and distance vision. Diabetes type II; 54/54 on star cancellation test. No evident ocular motor deficits. No restriction of neck movement. Reported difficulty reading and using computer; not driving due to visual impairment

B

male


58

right middle cerebral artery infarct

2

Left inferior quadrantanopia, good corrected acuity for near and distance. 54/54 on star cancellation test. No evident ocular motor deficits, but finds it more difficult to switch his gaze to the left. No restriction of neck movement. Reports visual problems as “a problem with his left side, which is intermittent, sporadic and pertinent when he is not paying attention”. He reports that he can be “impulsive and not good at acknowledging items on his left side”. Bumps into things on left when walking outside. Not driving because of his visual deficit. Research Occupational Therapist considered visual inattention in extra-personal space probable

C

male


65

right middle cerebral artery infarct

6

Left homonymous hemianopia, good corrected acuity for near and distance vision. No errors on star cancellation test. No evident ocular motor deficits. Range of neck movement is full for flexion and rotation, but reduced in extension. Reported that he “cannot see detail, it’s like a sheet of polythene is hanging down on the left-hand side. If I look straight ahead I cannot see on the left”.

D

female


73

small haemorrhage posterior to the right internal capsule.

2

Left homonymous hemianopia plus loss of peripheral vision to the right on perimetry. Good corrected acuity for near and distance vision. Some scarring in right eye following an infection. Errors to the left and right on star cancellation test (37/54) Difficulty with ocular pursuit, particularly when attempting to track to the left. Full range of rotational neck movements, but flexion and extension restricted by pain. Perceived visual problems: described as “seeing things but difficulty registering that I have seen them”. And not seeing clearly on the left side. Some difficulty with face recognition.

E

male


54

right sided occipital infarction

3

Left homonymous hemianopia, good corrected acuity for near and distance vision. 54/54 on star cancellation test. Reports lack of confidence in walking outside. Unable to drive is his main concern as it affects ability to work. Vision worse first thing in morning. Gets tired, takes longer than it used to do activities.

F

Female


84

Right middle cerebral artery infarct

4

Left sided homonymous hemianopia, and visual neglect. Only cancelling far right column of stars on star cancellation test. Good corrected acuity for near and distance vision, impaired eye movements, unable to direct gaze to left. Pre-stroke history of glaucoma; Unable to read; 2/3 range of neck rotation, left and right. Dependent in personal care, household tasks, transfers and meals and in moving around.

G

Female


75

Right cerebral haemorrhage

30

Left hemianopia, good corrected acuity for near and distance vision, possibly some left sided inattention - Achieved cut off score 51/54 (missing 3 stars to the left) on star cancellation. No evident ocular motor deficits. Restricted to 2/3 of neck rotation, left and right. Dependent in personal care, household tasks, transfers and meals and in moving around.

H

Female


66

Right cerebral haemorrhage

36

Left hemianopia and inattention to left; 50/54 (missed 4 stars to the right) on star cancellation. Good corrected acuity for near and distance vision. Astigmatism in left eye since childhood. Limited range of movement in her neck, particularly in extension, and rotation to the left. Complains she does not see well on both sides, she bumps into things and would not be able to cross roads.

I

Male


83

Right Occipital lobe, Posterior circulation infarct

2

Partial visual field loss on the left, with macular involvement. Achieved cut off score 51/54 (missing 3 stars to the left) on star cancellation. Difficulties with reading. Visual deficit impacts on kitchen tasks, and moving around the home, tends to bump into things. Complains he cannot see as he used to: can’t read, use the computer or attend to his financial affairs and is unable to drive.

  1   2


The database is protected by copyright ©dentisty.org 2016
send message

    Main page